Chapter 12
Gender, Politeness and Stereotypes
Women’s
language and confidence
Robin Lakoff, an American linguist, believed that women
language expressed uncertainty and lack of confidence. She also shifted the
focus of research from gender differences to syntax, semantics and style,
suggesting that women’s subordinate social status is reflected in the language
they are using.
She therefore suggested that women’s
speech is characterized by the following features:
Lexical fillers or hedges: “you know”,
“sort of”, “well”.
Tag questions
Rising intonation on declaratives “it’s
really good?”
Empty adjectives (meaning: using the
adjective alone like wonderful, pretty, and cute).
Precise color terms “aquamarine”
Intensifiers
Hypercorrect grammar
Super-polite forms like indirect requests
or euphemisms
Swear words avoidance
Emphatic stress “it was a WONDERFUL show”.
The research
Lakoff features were researched by
linguists, but such researches lacked linguistic expertise. An evidence of this
is the following:
1.
A
study regarded a statement like “will you open the door, please?” as an
imperative construction in the form of question, the thing which confuses form
and function.
2.
Another
study made a distinction between fillers and hedges, regarding “sort of” as a
hedge, and “well” as pause fillers like “um, eh, ah”.
3.
Some
studies could not identify the fundamental function of these features presented
by Lakoff, which is “expressing lack of confidence”. The list of Lakoff was not
arbitrary but rather unified. All the features express uncertainty or
tentativeness. But researches ignored this function and listed any forms that
produced difference between men and women.
(How this list is coherent?) The internal
cohesion of lakoff features can be illustrated by dividing them into two
groups:
a- Features that serve as hedging devices:
signal lack of confidence
b- Features that serve as boosting
devices: reflect the speaker’s anticipation that the addressee may remain
unconvinced.
So Lakoff suggested that women use hedging
devices to express uncertainty and use intensifying devices to persuade their
addressee. They use boosters because they believe that they will not be heard
or paid attention. Therefore, hedges and boosters are coherent because both
express uncertainty and lack of confidence.
The results
The research results were often
contradictory.
1- In some studies women were sometimes
found as using more tags than men, while in other studies men used more tags
than women.
2-Some studies found that there were no
gender differences in using hedges, while other studies found that women used
hedges three times more than men do.
3- Most studies claimed that women used
more boosters, but some studies did not.
4- A study recorded speech of male and
females in court, has found that a male witness used a language that is full of
features presented by Lakeoff. Therefore the study labeled such features as
“powerless forms” which are related to speech of powerless in general rather
than women.
5- Some studies also found out that
although women really used more boosters than men, they were not expressing
uncertainty or lack of confidence.
Lakoff linguistic features as politeness
devices
a-
Tags
are may function as a facilitative politeness device, providing the addressee with
an easy entrée into a conversation. This is used by teachers fro example to
facilitate the participation of a student, or by party hosts to give a topic to
the addressee to talk about. For example, a host addressing one of the guests,
saying “he has just came from Europe, haven’t you?” will allow this guest to
talk about his journey to Europe.
b-
A tag
may also soften a directive or a criticism. For example, a mother addressing
her child after he had emptied all the shopping items on the floor saying “that
was a daft thing to do, wasn’t it?” will soften her criticism of him, and will
consider his feeling.
c-
A tag
may function as confrontation and coercive devices. In such cases, a tag is
regarded as a booster not a hedge.
Based on Holmes
(1984), a distribution of tag use between men and women, Women are found to
focus more than men on the polite function of tags, while men are found to be
focusing more than women on tags for uncertainty.
Analyses which take
account of the function of the features of women speech often reveal women as
more facilitative conversationalists rather than uncertain and unconfident. By
using more standard forms, these women could be seen as responding positively
to their addresses by accommodating to their speech. Women’s greater use of
politeness devices can be another aspect of their consideration to the
addressee. Also, many of the features which characterize women language are
positive politeness devices expressing solidarity.
Politeness in
other cultures:
1- A study of a Mayan
community in Mexico, the women used more politeness forms than men, which
resembles the western norms. Men were using few politeness forms with each
other, so the men talk was plain and unmodified. In all other contexts everyone
used politeness devices. In this community, men’s talk could be seen as the
unusual variety rather than women’s talk.
2- In Malagasy, men
not women who modify and qualify their utterances, and who generally use
indirect language. Men are considered as the more polite speakers in this
community, which is in contrast with western norms. But this is due to social
roles of men and women in that community.
3- A study of
Samoan personal narratives has found that status was more important than gender
in accounting for the use of positive politeness devices. Statusful women with
a Samoan title used fewer politeness forms than young untitled men. On the other hand, titled Samoan men used the
highest frequency of negative politeness forms expressing social distance.
*The next section shows how context,
interaction, status and the meaning conveyed is relevant to the comparison
between men and women use of politeness devices.
1-
Interaction
There are many
features of interaction which differentiate the talk of women and men:
Interruptions: in same gender interactions in a
context of a coffee bar, interruptions are distributed equally between
speakers. In cross-gender interactions, all interruptions are from males. In
other contexts, it is found that men interrupt others more than women do, and
that parents interrupt children, and daughters are interrupted the most. A study of pre-schoolers found out that some
boys start practicing this strategy for dominating the talk at a very early
age.
Feedback: women provide more encouraging feedback
(hmm, mm, aha) to their conversational patterns. A study in New Zealand has
examined the positive feedback distribution in casual contexts, and found out
that women gave four times as much of positive feedback to their addressees
than men do.
One researcher
also found out that women students were more likely than men to develop the
ideas of a previous speaker rather than challenge them.
In general, women
are found to be cooperative conversationalists, while men are found as more
competitive and less supportive.
Why?
2- Gossip
Gossip describes
the kind of relaxed in-group talk that goes on between people in informal
contexts. In western societies, it is considered as a characteristic of women’s
interaction whose function is to affirm solidarity and maintain the social
relationships between the women involved.
It focuses on personal experiences and personal relationships, non
personal problems and feelings, and may include the criticism of others. In
gossip sessions, they provide sympathetic responses focusing on the affective
rather than the referential content.
As for men, a
study of men working in a bakery has found that the topics discussed tend to
focus on things and activities rather than feelings. The men gossip includes
linguistic features like long pauses, responses challenged the previous
speaker. The men criticized each other constantly, and change the topic
abruptly. Their talks contrasted completely with the cooperative, supportive
and agreeing and coherent one of women.
However, there is
a variation in the western communities too. Not all men and women behave the
same way in western communities. For example, in Malagasy, women’s speech is
more direct than that of men. Women take a more confrontational role, and
handle bargaining in the market place. Women
not men deal with family disagreements and arguments.
The construction of Gender
Approaching gender identity as a
construction is useful because:
1- It accounts for the cases where women adapt to masculine
contexts, and men to feminine contexts.(How)Women in police force construct a masculine identity by wearing
bulky sweaters, not smiling and talking roughly, while men in cloths markets or
beauty salons are constructing a feminine identity by avoiding swear words, and
encouraging the customer to talk. They also tend to gossip.
2- Narratives of personal experience allow one to construct social
and gender identities. (How)
A woman who encounters a personal story to one of her women friends would
construct a gendered identity. The example no. 20 and 21 illustrates that the
woman expressed the identity of a “loving mother” or a “dutiful daughter”. The
discourse style would be full of hedges and pragmatic particles like “you
know”, “you see”, as well as feminine adjectives as “cute” or “little”. In
other context, the same woman, working as a senior manager, would construct a
more contestive identity in her work place by challenging everything she
disagrees with. She therefore constructs a less-conformist gender identity. The
discourse style in this case will include less hedges and pragmatic particles.
The characters talked about in the woman’s story would be also given gendered
identities. For example, the woman’s daughter was given the identity of a sweet
little girl. Therefore, narratives are a means of expressing gender identities,
and every phonological, lexical and syntactic selection conveys social
information. This is why women are against sexist languages.
Sexist language
Sexist language is one example of the way
a culture conveys its values from one group to another and from one generation
to the next. It encodes stereotyped attitudes to women and men.
In principle, the study of sexist language
is concerned with the way language expresses both women and men.
In practice, research in this area has
concentrated on the ways in which language conveyed negative attitudes on
women.
There are a number of ways that suggest
English as discriminating against women due to the derogatory forms related to
women which are more than those related to men, and due to the English
metaphors that describe women negatively more than those related to men.
- Negative animal imagery related to women like “bitch”, “cow” in comparison to “wolf”
which is related to men. “Birds” indicate feathery brains, and “chick” and
“kitty” which indicate powerlessness.
- Food imagery:
Women are also described in food imagery which is insulting.
- Male forms are regarded as the unmarked form (the base). Many words reflect women as
deviant or subordinate group. For example, the English morphology generally
takes the male form as the base, and adds a suffix to the end of the word to
signal femininity: lion-lioness, usher-usherette, author- authoress. The adding of the female suffix is seen as
conveying the message that women are deviant or abnormal.
- Generic structures
also support the claim that English marginalizes women. For example, the use of
“Man” as generic forms supports the claim that
English renders women as invisible. It is no longer accepted by English
speakers because this meaning has indicated masculine meaning. It is also
confusing, because when we say “man loves to hunt” we don’t know if the
sentence means man in masculine sense or the generic sense. The word man is
also associated with male images even when it is used generically. For example,
a study instructed college students to select pictures that express political
man, industrial man, social man, and most of the pictures were men pictures.
Generic “he” also raises the same questions. Grammarians used to deal
with such a problem first by proposing many pronouns that are “genre-neutral”.
Bisexual pronouns like per, ou, hiser were proposed since the eighteenth
century.
Writers and journalists are now avoiding
the use of generic “he” and “man”. An American study of magazines found a
dramatic drop in the use of generic forms.
Some writers use “he” and “she” in
alternative chapters or in alternate paragraphs.
Singular “they” is used in many writing of
old authors like Bernard show and Shakespeare.
However, in the nineteenth century the
parliament passed a resolution by which in all acts the masculine gender shall
be deemed and taken to include females.
Glossary:
Pragmatic
particles: ألفاظ تستخدم أثناء الكلام وتدل على المعرفة القريبة
بين المتحدثين
Ex: the phrase “You
know” implies an attempt to maintain an already close relationship with the
person being addressed, to simulate shared views – or to establish such a
relationship.
Sexist
language: لغة تمييزية بين الرجال والنساء
Gender-neutral محايد الجنس
Imagery الصور البلاغية
Intensifying
devices أدوات تشديدية
Hedges ألفاظ تقال للحيطة “في حالة عدم التأكد"
Boosters ألفاظ تقال للتأكيد على الكلام
To pass a
resolution يصدر قرار
To account for يعلل
No comments:
Post a Comment